|
WEEK 3
RMA 2013 Lecture 3 Notes
SLIDE 2
Background to Effects Research
• If we see an audience as an object, then that object has no real power (Sullivan, 2013)
• Mass audiences are faceless anonymous collections of millions of people (end 19C)
• Use of statistics to understand populations used by government and business (early 20C)
• New forms of mass media (radio, film) supports notions of mass audiences
• All media research looks for effects!
SLIDE 3
Hypodermic needle theory:
• Brett Lamb http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Qt5MjBlvGcY
• Beauty and advertising http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=JAW4LIFYFng
• The pron effect http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=R8ptP3qFIxA
• 12 yr old Vajazzling http://www.mirror.co.uk/news/uk-news/girls-having-bikini-waxes-vajazzling-1787328
SLIDE 4
Effects theories
In a very useful summary of historical shifts in effects research,
McQuail (1994: 328– 32) describes four phases which have developed over
time in response to changing social environments, each building
incrementally on what has gone before, including both incorporating and
challenging the ‘old’ order. This is not to suggest that theory
necessarily progresses in a neat and orderly fashion, since it is as
likely to be cyclical as linear. But it is to indicate that there are
key moments in the development of a field, which can be understood in
the context of an organizing framework for understanding social
phenomena.
1st phase — media as all-powerful
This research phase spans the turn of the twentieth century to the
1930s and the assumptions during that early period were that the mass
media were highly influential and operated as modes of persuasion, if
not control, in a one-way direction, from the economic and political
elites to ‘the people’. However, this phase was more inferential and
ideological than empirically driven, resting on the popularity of
certain media forms and an assumption that the audiences for those
messages were generally naïve and susceptible to covert propaganda.
2nd phase — challenge to the all-powerful media model
The move towards empirically oriented studies began as a challenge to
the earlier theoretical ideas about media and effect and developed a
more sophisticated research agenda which looked at differences in media
format, genre and content (see, for example, Blumer 1933). Much
research in this period, from around the 1930s up to the late 1950s,
looked at the media’s potential to influence voters through political
campaigns, at the effects of certain narratives on deviant behaviours
(for example the moral panics constructed in the media around juvenile
delinquency — see Cohen 1972) and at the media as sources of
information. What became clear in many of the studies of this period
were the number of variables which needed to be factored into analyses,
including personal characteristics, but also the kinds of exposure to
other sources of information. This phase ended with a strong sense of
the media’s place in any number of influences, suggesting that the
media did probably, have some impact but they were only one part of a
pre-existing social, economic and political structure in which
individuals function. By thinking of the media as part of a larger
picture, it then becomes difficult to isolate the precise influence of
the media away from other possible sources. (The 'no effect') model.)
3rd phase — powerful media revisited
As with any theory, no sooner is it written than someone wants to
challenge it and so it was with the ‘no-effect’ model. By the 1960s,
researchers were questioning the basis on which claims of no-effect had
been made and began to seek out ways in which to demonstrate effect
(see Lang and Lang 1981). But this time, the tools for analysis had
moved on from a simple stimulus-response model which could measure
immediate effect, which characterized the 1st phase, to ones which
sought to identify longer-term effects, subtle shifts in understanding,
diverse contexts and motivations for attending to media outputs. This
phase also saw an interest in the more structural aspects of media
production such as journalistic practice, media ownership and wider
questions of political economy, as well as looking at less visible
notions such as ideology. Crucially, the development of television in
this era prompted a renewed interest in the power of media persuasion
(Elliot 1972). A particular and popular theory which emerged during
this time was Gerbner’s cultivation analysis theory which explored the
relationships between audience exposure to TV (especially those of
heavy ‘users’) and their beliefs and behaviours, specifically to
identify the ‘acculturation’ effect of TV on viewers (Gerbner, 1967).
Importantly, Gerbner insisted that it was the cumulative effect of
watching hours, days, weeks, months and years of TV which ‘cultivated’
the effect, not simply watching a few violent TV shows or hearing
periodic outbursts of swearing.
4th phase — negotiating media meaning
This latest (but perhaps not last!) phase, which began in the late
1970s and early 1980s, has been characterized by an interest in the
ways in which media messages are constructed and offered up to
audiences for their consumption and how audiences either accept the
(dominant) ways in which texts are encoded, or reject them or negotiate
them (see Chapter 2 for an elaboration of the encoding/decoding model).
In other words, audience research began to focus on an examination of
what audiences did with media, rather than what media did to audiences,
emphasizing agency rather than passivity.
Studies such as Morley’s (1980) work on the Nationwide programme and
Hobson’s (1982) work on Crossroads made clear that there were many
different ways in which audiences understood and interpreted media
texts. Moreover, what also became clear was that audiences used media
texts in a variety of ways — as ways in which to ‘practice’ other ways
of being or as the basis for workplace discussions.
In other words, media consumption had a utility over and above the
immediate consumption of a particular programme or news item. This
phase also saw the further development of alternative methodological
practices, moving away from quantitative approaches towards more
nuanced understandings of audiences’ lived experiences, necessitating a
more qualitative approach which would enable the personal
meaning-making process and belief structures to be teased out (see Hall
1980; Morley 1980). These new methodologies included strategies such as
observing how families watch TV and looking at who has control of the
remote.
McQuail argues that what this phase contributes to our
understanding of effects is that there are two broad imperatives: that
'the media construct social formations and history itself by framing
images of reality (in fiction as well as news) in a predictable and
patterned way; and that people in audiences construct for themselves
their own view of social reality and their place in it, in interaction
with the symbolic constructions offered by the media’ (McQuail 1994:
331).
Thus the influence of the media will vary along the continuum of
all-nothing, depending on where we, as individual consumers, are
situated along that line in terms of our own relationship to the
message, our experience, our background and our beliefs.
Whilst McQuail (1994) offers a useful way into understanding shifts in
audience effect theory, his is by no means the only model available.
Perse (2001), for example, argues that looking at the types rather than
the extent of effect is a different way of thinking about the
phenomenon, although she makes the point that such models can provide
only partial explanations of the highly complex process of affect.
5th Phase - 2005 Social Media changes mass media theory
SLIDE 5
For Perse (2001: 51), then, there are four alternate models of effect,
which she describes as: direct, conditional, cumulative and
cognitive/transactional:
Direct effects — these are generally short-term and testable and assume
a passive audience unable to challenge ‘hidden’ media messages or an
audience unconscious of the impact of media content on their behaviour
(see later discussion on violence and affect).
Conditional effects — these are effects which are contingent on the
predispositions, personal attributes and belief systems of individual
audience members and allow the audience active agency to decide which
aspects of a given media text they are willing to accept.
Cumulative effects — this model suggests that despite the potential for
individual negotiation and decision to consume some but not other
messages, the media is saturated with certain kinds of message where
repetition of theme subverts conscious agency.
Cognitive-transactional effects — this model sees media effects as the
consequence of individual cognitive responses towards media content and
uses the concept of ‘priming’ to suggest that audiences are primed to
watch, remember and thus potentially be influenced by, certain kinds of
content rather than others. The transactional aspect of the model
refers to the fact that both media content and audience characteristics
are important in understanding media effect.
Whilst Perse presents an interesting analysis of media effect, the
relationship of her typology to more familiar renditions of media
effect is not substantially different except for her insistence on
understanding effect through the combined lens of both content and
audience profile, which is powerfully persuasive. Most studies tend to
look either at content or at audience, so a model which combines both
these elements can provide, in principle, a much more comprehensive
analysis.
Yet other contemporary researchers are moving beyond (or perhaps
alongside) the orthodoxy of identifying audience effect as constituting
either passive incorporation or active resistance by constructing a new
paradigm, that of spectacle/performance.
Some proponents of this new audiencethink, such as Abercrombie and
Longhurst (1998), believe that ‘the audience’ should be set free from
the confines of medium and genre. Instead, the ‘everydayness’ of being
an audience member and thus witnessing a variety of performances (that
is via consuming TV , newspapers, films, radio shows and so on) should
be understood more intuitively and holistically in terms of our/their
forms of identity with the entire mediascape which has become our
ordinary, media-saturated world.
Methodologically, Abercrombie and Longhurst are arguing for a research
paradigm which is essentially ethnographic and which takes a much more
comprehensive approach to the notion of audience and effect than has
been attempted hitherto. Of course, new theory about media influence
(and the lack thereof) is developing all the time, but what remains
clear is that many people, advertisers and politicians among them, do
believe that the media have an influence so that the argument is less
about if and more about how much.
And as Carey (1988) points out, mass media are an easy target to blame
in times of significant social shifts or developments of new social
phenomena, although the extent to which the media construct those
social shifts or simply report on their progress continues to be a
primary point of research contention. But at crucial times in history,
the media are influential in informing publics about the progress of,
say, a war or other armed conflict, but it is the extent to which the
media construct and therefore influence what becomes ‘the agenda’ which
is important (but difficult) to map. But we can say that most media
scholars would cede some effect to mass media, but the point here is
that the media have effects simply by their existence: suggesting they
have power, on the other hand, suggests an effect which is altogether
more deliberate than just ‘being there’
SLIDE 6
The problem of media violence
The way in which media such as newspapers and television deal with and
treat issues of crime and violence has been an abiding preoccupation
for media researchers, principally because of the potential for copycat
behaviour by audiences, especially children (Schlesinger 1991; Paik and
Comstock 1994; Wilson et al. 1997). Two of the earliest studies were
carried out in the mid-1950s by Head (1954) and Smythe (1954), both of
which argued that acts of violence were three times as frequent in
programmes aimed at children than in mainstream programming. Other
similar studies followed throughout the subsequent decades with
repeated attempts to map patterns and trends in the volume, if not the
actual impact, of TV violence. Whilst early researchers shied away from
an overly prescriptive definition of what a violent act actually
comprised for counting purposes, the scholar whose body of work on
‘counting’ violence effects which are contingent on the
predispositions, personal attributes and belief systems of individual
audience members and allow the audience active agency to decide which
aspects of a given media text they are willing to accept.
But at crucial times in history, the media are influential in informing
publics about the progress of a war or other armed conflict, but it is
the extent to which the media construct and therefore influence what
becomes ‘the agenda’ which is important (but difficult) to map. We can
say that most media scholars would cede some effect to mass media, but
the point here is that the media have effects simply by their
existence: suggesting they have power, on the other hand, suggests an
effect which is altogether more deliberate than just ‘being there’.
The expression of injurious or lethal force had to be credible and real
in the symbolic terms of the drama. Humorous and even farcical violence
can be credible and real, even if it has a presumable comic effect.
(Gerbner 1972: 31) Gerbner’s work and that of other US colleagues
throughout the 1980s and 1990s (for example, Gerbner et al. 1995; Cole
1996) suggests that the trend at the close of the twentieth century was
a decreasing amount of violence, in simple volume terms, on terrestrial
TV but an increase in volume across nonterrestrial programming. Whilst
the precise reasons for this apparent pattern are not immediately
apparent, US-produced programmes appear to be significantly ahead in
their violence quotient when compared with the output of other
industrialized countries (see Takeuchi et al. 1995). In a UCLA study of
home video rental content, researchers found that in the monitoring
period 1995– 97, more than 50 per cent of rentals had so much violence
that concern would have been raised if any of them had been broadcast
uncut on prime-time TV (UCLA Center for Communication Policy 1998).
SLIDE 7
• Most domestic violence are men -> women
• Stranger rape much less common than acquaintances
• Murder is not the most common crime
• Black people more likely to be victims
• Older people more likely to be victims
• Domestic violence accounted for 25% of all violent crime, and 33% of all murders in USA
• These stats - not the portrayal of violence on TV and movies
• http://youtu.be/DHRwo48twyE Book of Eli
SLIDE 8
The appalling events at Columbine, where 19 children and teachers were
killed, was reported by most media as the tragic ‘consequences’ of two
adolescent killers’ obsession with violent video games. The following
headline makes clear who ‘society’ blames: ‘School massacre families to
sue creators of violent games’ (The Independent, 7 June 1999: 3).
James Bulger case
Such a response mirrors that provoked by the killing of a 2-year-old
child, James Bulger, by two adolescent boys who were also reported to
have been fascinated with violent material. In that instance, the film
Childs Play 3 was linked directly with the killing. The brutal death of
James Bulger in 1998 prompted the Home Office to pursue yet more
research into the impact of violent media, culminating in a study
which, not surprisingly, did suggest such a link, although it had to
stop short of making the link directly causal. The headline, though, is
somewhat less scrupulous: ‘Film violence link to teenage crime: new
twist to video nasty debate — ‘vulnerable’ young people may be
influenced by screen killings’ (The Guardian, 8 January 1998). The
salient words here are ‘vulnerable’ and ‘may’, both of which are
contestable and controversial.
But we do need to understand that concern with viewers’ propensity to
unthinkingly imitate media violence or be depraved or corrupted by it,
is nothing new and efforts to prevent violent media content in books,
magazines and newspapers have been ongoing since at least the late
nineteenth century (Saunders 1996). Now, as then, one of the principal
arguments against restricting and further regulating media content is
that of freedom of speech (Gunter 2002), often linked with exhortations
that consumers have the power to switch off or not read or listen to
offensive material. In a recent, industry-funded project into effect,
the researchers insist that it is misleading to point the finger of
guilt at all broadcasters for portraying excessive violence since the
worst excesses were mostly seen on pay-per-view channels or at times
when children were unlikely to be watching (Gunter 2003). Whilst it is
tempting to simply dismiss such research as mere industry propaganda,
the differentiation between different parts of the industry does need
to be acknowledged. In any case, the efficacy of strategies to limit
vulnerable viewers’ access to violent material in response to concerns
expressed by society and government are hard to assess. The criteria
for classifying films, for example, are persistently challenged by the
industry and on the grounds that members of the classification board
are out of touch with reality and with public understandings of taste,
decency and what is ‘acceptable’ material for different aged audiences.
A study found that the effectiveness of V-chip technology is severely
limited by the industry’s reluctance to label programmes in ways which
accurately reflect their adult/violence/sexual content (Kunkel et al.
2002). Violence as (kidz)play
Today
The continuing debate around copycat violence is often complicated by
the contradictory nature of research findings in the areas, so that for
every study showing a cause-effect relation, another suggests the
opposite conclusions, thus producing an entirely inconclusive evidence
base. This is not least because the different foci, context,
sample-base, age cohort and exposure times in different studies has
resulted in substantially different analyses and interpretation. It is
therefore highly problematic to try and develop either comparative
perspectives or to reach credible conclusions. Just as many researchers
believe there is a relationship between violent-content video games and
aggression (Irwin and Gross 1995; Ballard and Lineberger 1999) as
believe the opposite (Cooper and Mackie 1986; Graybill et al. 1985;
Scott 1995). Even reviewers of the literature cannot agree on the
existence or not of causal effect (Dill and Dill 1998; Griffiths 1999),
although some, like Freedman (2002) uncompromisingly assert that there
is no scientific evidence to suggest effect. He argues that the fact
that violent video games have proliferated at the same time as violent
crime has decreased makes it improbable that one causes the other,
since the effect appears to work in the ‘wrong’ direction.
SLIDE 9
The evidence re media violence
The problem with the incidence of media violence is that it is
completely contradicted by the statistical evidence; even in the USA,
crime rates are going down rather than up (Bureau of Justice Statistics
1998). The point is, why does the volume of violent material on
television matter? Why are we interested in measuring it and mapping
its contours? Well, obviously, it is because there is a belief that
there is some kind of cause and effect relationship going on, between
watching violence and ‘doing’ violence, between a viewer consuming
violent TV programmes and then perpetrating violent acts against real
people in real life. It is odd, then, that the early studies of TV
violence were almost entirely oriented towards content, counting
specific acts across the television landscape, identifying which genres
or media were most culpable in their displays of televisual aggression
and which genres were performing better or worse over time, rather than
investigating viewer response and therefore affect. Public convictions
that violent media content contributes to violence in society are
supported by anecdotal reports of criminals’ media use, naïve beliefs
in the connections between crime rates and media violence, media
reports of ‘copycat’ crime and the publicized reports of some highly
visible research. (Perse 2001: 199)
The cause and effect relation was often implicit in such studies but
was rarely ‘tested’ for its strength with real audiences or even by
mapping trends in the volume of TV violence against trends in the
volume of real-life violence. Perhaps part of this reluctance to
seriously engage with the potentiality of ‘real’ affect has been an
acknowledgement that TV violence is not the same as the real thing
which happens messily inside people’s lives (Fiske and Hartley 1978;
Lichter et al. 1994). The violence and terror we see on television bear
little or no relationship to their actual occurrence . . . television
violence is an overkill of ‘happy violence’ — swift, cool, effective,
without tragic consequences and in other ways divorced from real life
and crime statistics. (Gerbner 1995: 71, 73) Most acts of domestic
violence, for example, are perpetrated against women by men they know,
and stranger rape is substantially less frequent than rape by a
husband, partner or boyfriend. More than twenty years ago, it was
estimated that between three and four million American women were
battered by their partners (Stark et al. 1981) and global predictions
more than a decade ago were that two-thirds of all married women would
be battered at some time in their married lives (Stout 1991). In the
UK, in 2001– 02, one in four women experienced domestic violence
(British Crime Survey for England and Wales 2002). Domestic violence
accounted for 25 per cent of all violent crime and one third of all
murders in 2002 (Tweedale 2003): also in that year there were 635,000
reported incidents of domestic violence in England and Wales and women
were the victims in 81 per cent of cases (Gorna 2003).
Other crime facts which run counter to common-sense myths are that
murder is not the most common crime, that black people are much more
likely to be victims than perpetrators and that older people are much
less likely to be assaulted on the street than young people. This is
the actuality of crime trends, but television shows us exactly the
reverse of these facts because TVland is about excitement and drama
with neatly closed ends: only rarely are we offered glimpses of the
real impact of violence on people’s real lives and relationships. To
unpack this a little and focus on the media’s portrayal of crimes
against women, Myers (1997) argues that one of the ways in which the
media simplify what are very complex issues is in their persistent use
of two contradictory frames: Madonna and whore. In this schema, women
are either innocent victims of male lust and violence or guilty of
incitement by their own behaviour and conduct. The repetitive use of
these two central motifs or what Myers (1997: 9) describes as the ‘male
supremacist ideology’ produces a powerful social lesson. Women are
warned through these reporting mechanisms about the limits of
‘acceptable’ female behaviour and the likely outcome (rape/ murder) of
their behavioural transgression. This setting up of a binary opposition
of good girl/bad girl finds resonance with feminist notions of
patriarchy and the supposed ‘place’ of women in society (Soothill and
Walby 1991; Benedict 1992; Meyers 1994).
The utility of thinking about violent media as a set of characteristics
as suggested by Donnerstein et al. is neatly encapsulated in the
findings from National Television Violence Studies (see Wilson et al.
1997, 1998) which suggested that viewers perceiving justification for
an act of aggression had an impact on subsequent violent behaviour.
Thus watching ‘justified’ violence appeared to give ‘permission’ to
viewers to be aggressive themselves, whereas watching unjustified acts
may have the opposite effect. In work with children and violent TV,
Krcmar and Cooke (2001) argue that age (and therefore the range of
experiences on which viewers have to draw) has a significant impact on
ideas about the rightness or wrongness of violent behaviour. Young
children are more likely to see unpunished aggressive acts as being
‘better’ than punished acts, whereas older children are more likely to
see provoked acts of violence as more acceptable than unprovoked ones.
In an experimental study with 10– 12-year-old children in the USA,
images which showed an armed criminal being killed (that is, attracting
negative consequences for violent action) were a stronger inhibitor to
imitative behaviour than scenes where the armed perpetrator ‘got away
with it’ (Bernhardt et al. 2001).
Successive opinion polls have shown that a majority of Americans
believe that the influence of television on the incidence of crime is
either important or critical (US Department of Justice 1994: 222; cited
in Fowles 1999: 13). Such third-person effects (Davison 1983; McLeod et
al. 2001), that is believing that something affects other people, but
not oneself, are particularly strong is this contested area of effect,
and Hoffner et al. (2001) show that audiences believe they are much
less influenced by violent images than other people. Similarly, Duck
and Mullin (1995) found that viewers rejected being influenced by
negative content (oriented towards violence, sexism and racism) whilst
accepting their own openness to ‘good’ content, for example, public
service announcements.
Video games have been especially targeted as the focus of much
audience-based research relating to children and affect, largely
because of their popularity, the sociable context of interaction (that
is, often with other children) and the fact that they can be
experienced with a VCR and are therefore more accessible than games
requiring computer facilities. The violent content of video games has
prompted concern about affect for more than ten years and as recently
as 2000, the mayor of Indianapolis spearheaded a campaign to ban
children under 18 years old from playing violent video games unless
accompanied by an adult (cited in Sherry 2001: 410).
Whilst links are often made between watching violence on TV and playing
violent video games, that is, that the impact on the audience is more
or less the same, there are very specific differences between the two
modes which require more sensitive as the trigger for a similar kind of
assault carried out by a group of girls just days after the film was
broadcast on TV . However, the legal case which was brought by the
mother of the victim against NBC (the broadcaster who aired the film)
was dropped after it was revealed that the ringleader had not watched
the film.
Another example Fowles cites is from 1993, when a five-year-old boy set
fire to his family’s trailer home in Ohio, killing his two-year-old
sister. His mother blamed the Beavis and Butt-head show for inciting
the child to set fires, but reporters investigating the tragedy found
not only that the boy’s trailer home was not hooked up to cable — and
that he could not therefore have been a regular viewer — but that the
trailer park itself was not wired for cable. The point that Fowles is
trying to make is that the adult public is keen to condemn TV for its
routinized portrayal of violence, and programmes get scapegoated for
their provocative material when in fact, ‘the total number of
antisocial acts directly attributable to television entertainment
antics must be minuscule’ (Fowles 1999: 3).
He also points to the abiding contradiction which is the cause-effect
conundrum relating to TV violence: whilst individuals believe that
there is too much violence on TV and that exposure does have an impact
on violence in society, so they simultaneously get their daily fix of
TV mayhem as if they are not also part of the problem. For Fowles, this
contradictory behaviour can best be understood by way of an
individual’s need to ‘act out’ aggressively (in an aggressive world),
but within the safe confines of the TV set, a desire which is
encouraged by the wider community:
SLIDE 10
Media accuracy and bias
• Journalists often reproduce bias surrounding various issues, forgetting to check sources
• Attitudes to beauty, obesity, alcohol, drugs,
education, politics, boat people… are created and maintained by media
• Agenda-setting role leads to mass audience paranoia in some cases – HRT reports
http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/181726.php
• Media is subject to PR messages from big business
http://www.universaldrugstore.com/news/tamiflu/is-tamiflu-better-than-relenza/
SLIDE 11
The effect/affect conundrum
What we hope we have shown is the endurance and tenacity with which
proponents and detractors of ‘effect’ continue to argue about the
cause-effect relation within mass media research. However, despite the
clear lack of demonstrable and verifiable evidence that a cause and
effect relationship does exist between the sender and the receiver,
supporters of the ‘negative effect’ thesis will often make very firm
statements about causality. In several US studies, all make clear that
the (high) volume of violent material on TV across all genres and
channels, has dangerous consequences for society in terms of imitative
behaviours, especially amongst children, and an increasing sense of
social menace and fear (see Smith and Boyson 2002; Smith et al. 2002;
Wilson et al. 2002).
But all these studies focus exclusively on programme content, not on
audience perceptions, so their strong statements on the cause-effect
relation can only actually be speculative and derived from their own
private beliefs and concerns. Despite this limitation, though, these
researchers are still able to claim that, ‘In spite of the lively
debates [about the impact of TV] that still occur . . . social science
research that has accumulated over 40 years reveals quite clearly that
television violence can contribute to aggressive behaviour in viewers’
(Smith et al. 2002: 84). The use of the word ‘can’ here both renders
the statement completely empty but is also small enough for the reader
to miss if she is already predisposed towards believing a positive
association.
But no such incontrovertible evidence actually exists, and Vine (1997:
126), amongst others, wants to question the casual ease with which many
media researchers confidently assert its existence. He argues
persuasively that what is at issue is not if media messages have an
effect — they clearly do, however involuntarily — but rather ‘which
kinds of effect occur [and] how they are brought about — and whether
the outcomes are to be properly judged as harmful’. Crucially, he
cautions against seeing one element — violent TV content — as the
primary (external) causal factor in antisocial or dangerous behaviour
when a variety of both exogenous and endogenous effects will also be in
play at any one time.
Not only are effects circumscribed by personal characteristics such as
age, gender, ethnicity and class — with gender attributes particularly
salient — but issues of genre are also important. For example, several
studies suggest that broadcasting ‘real’ violence in the form of news
reports of war is far more damaging to psychological health than
watching cartoon renditions of aggression (see Cantor 1994). In
Firmstone’s (2002: 49) review of the literature on viewer attitudes
towards violent media content in factual TV , she found a number of
characteristics which influence perception:
a) closeness — viewers were more disturbed by violence where they could identify with the victim;
b) certainty — viewers were less likely to be shocked if they knew how the situation would end and understood the context;
c) justice — viewers tolerated high levels of violence if they thought the victim ‘deserved it’;
d) sufficiency — viewers were disturbed by programmes using excessive violence to make a point.
One outcome of increasing levels of crime and violence on television is
an amplification of a climate of fear amongst the public which does not
reflect any kind of statistical or even experiential reality.
Successive studies in the UK, for example, show that the public’s fear
of crime and of being victims of criminal activity is unrelated to the
actual incidence of crime (see annual studies such as the British Crime
Survey). But of course, there is the usual chicken and egg situation in
play here: are ‘real-crime’ shows proliferating because of public
demand, or are audiences watching these shows because they are on?
Either way, there is a morbid fascination about watching such shows,
akin to rubber-necking at accident sites.
The media cannot really be thought of as an undifferentiated mass,
since TV must be considered separately from the press, radio from the
Internet, not just because their messages could be (and often are)
different, but because their purposes are as different as their modes
of address and reception. Even within the single category of television
or print, there are any number of nuanced differences in terms of
differential ‘affect’, between fiction and documentary, between tabloid
and broadsheet, between afternoon and midnight, between adults and
children.
Ironically, it could be that our fear of crime manifests itself in a
greater rather than lesser desire to consume crime-related material,
especially ‘reality-based’ and reconstruction-focused series such as
Crimewatch UK or America’s Most Wanted (see Gunter 1987). This is
because of the paradox inherent in watching such shows, which can
simultaneously exacerbate and reduce fears for personal safety, as
Schlesinger et al. (1992) have demonstrated persuasively in their work.
In their study with women viewers, they found that Crimewatch reduced
some women’s fear but increased that of others.
SLIDE 12
What about Advertising?
• If the media did not cause consumers to go out and
buy stuff, why is billions of dollars spent on advertising?
• Is behaving in a violent manner so different to buying clothes or food?
• Why do governments spend millions on health campaigns through the media?
SLIDE 13 & SLIDE 14
Successful PR Campaigns
Edward Bernays, “torches of freedom” 1929
Barack Obama’s 2008 election
SLIDE 15
Media effects on sexuality
• http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1070813/
• http://www.lifesitenews.com/news/archive//ldn/2005/jul/05070604
• Issue largely ignored by researchers:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/116/Supplement_1/303.full
SLIDE 16
How do we resist the media?
• Realise that the media are not there to help us
• Understand that status, profit, and power are the bases of all democratic societies
• What we see is not always reality, but a reflection of what already exists in other media copy
• Try to not mimic American culture in all its forms
• Try to educate ourselves by seeing through the hype, the sensationalism, the falsehoods posing as facts
• Try to adopt an individual view on difficult issues, not just parrot the media position
|
|